check
Publications | Prof. Eyal Pe'er

Publications

Working Paper
Peer, E., Rotschild, D. & Gordon, A., Working Paper. Platform over procedure: Online platforms that pre-vet participants yield higher data quality without sacrificing diversity . Available at: . Publisher's VersionAbstract

After decades of using mostly college students, researchers in psychology and social sciences gradually moved online to more cost-effective and flexible audiences. First, to samples of online convenience (CraigsList, Facebook), and then to sophisticated markets like Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk). Now, advanced platforms (such as CloudResearch and Prolific) promise researchers higher data quality using ex-ante vetting and controls on their participants’ pool. We systematically examine the impact of these ex-ante controls, and their effects on various measures of data quality, and compare them to standard ex-post approaches of attention checks (in randomized positions). We find that ex-ante controls outperform ex-post checks both on process measures of attention, honesty, and reliability, as well as on outcome measures of replicability. Additionally, while samples from platforms with ex-ante vetting show key differences in representation from the general population, they are still more representative than a major university lab pool, and no less representative than samples that are not ex-ante vetted. We conclude that pre-vetting of participants on the platform level should become the desired standard for online psychological and behavioral research, and could serve as a sufficient condition for ensuring a sample’s data quality ex-ante.  

Peer, E. et al., Working Paper. Honesty Pledges: the Effects of Involvement and Identification Over Time. Available at: . Publisher's VersionAbstract

Authorities and managers often rely on individuals’ and businesses’ self-reports, and employ various kinds of veracity statements, honesty pledges or oaths to ensure they do not over-claim payments, benefits, or other resources. While some research show honesty pledges can reduce dishonesty, others have provided mixed, and sometimes even contradictory, findings. We argue that understanding and predicting when honesty pledges are effective has been obstructed due to variations in the operationalizations of honesty pledges in previous research. Specifically, we identify that previous studies varied in whether and how the pledge asks individuals to identify (e.g., by ID, signature or name) and how much involvement, if at all, the pledge requires from the individual. In four pre-registered studies (N>5,000), we systematically examine these elements of a pledge to find that increasing involvement of pledgers (by having them copy the text of the pledge) is often more effective than those that only require identification. In contrast, pledges that only require individuals to read and agree are mostly ineffective. Moreover, we find that the effects of high-involvement pledges persist both over time and both after a short delay between the pledge and the opportunity to cheat. Together, these results contribute both theoretically to the understanding of the mechanisms underlying different honesty pledges as well as offer practical advice to managers and policymakers on how to effectively prevent or reduce dishonesty in self-reports.

2021
Peer, E. & Egelman, S., 2021. The Key to Getting People to Be Safer Online: Make the Appeals More Personal. Wall Street Journal. Available at: . Publisher's Version
Gamliel, E. & Peer, E., 2021. When two wrongs make a right: The efficiency-consumption gap under separate vs. joint evaluations. Judgment and Decision-Making, 16(1), pp.94-113. Available at: . Publisher's VersionAbstract
The MPG illusion and the time-saving bias both show that people misjudge the gains from increases in efficiency or speed, because people falsely believe that efficiency and speed are linearly related to consumption (e.g., gallons of fuel or journey time). This efficiency-consumption gap (ECG) has been demonstrated consistently in various situations. In parallel, people have also been found to show a diminished sensitivity to increases in magnitudes when judged under separate vs. joint evaluation modes (SE vs. JE). We show that these “two wrongs can make a right”: when people judge efficiency upgrades under SE mode, their subjective judgments follow a concave curve that closely resembles the curvilinear pattern of efficiency upgrades, making their preferences (artificially) less biased than they are under JE. In two studies we show that when asked for their willingness-to-pay (WTP) for upgrading products or services in two (a smaller vs. a larger) upgrade options, WTPs are less different in SE vs. JE modes. This means that people are exhibiting lower sensitivity to the upgrade size under SE which leads to a de-biasing effect. We show that because JE follow a linear trend, it yields biased preferences for efficiency measures, but not for consumption measures. In contrast, SE yield biased preferences for consumption, but not for efficiency measures.
Peer, E. et al., 2021. Data Quality of Platforms and Panels for Online Behavioral Research. Behavior Research Methods. Available at: Your article is available as 'Online First': http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13428-021-01694-3. Publisher's VersionAbstract

Researchers are increasingly using online audiences to conduct studies and surveys, but there is still considerable uncertainty about the quality of the available audience options. We first engage with the research community to explore which attributes of data quality are most critical to researchers: comprehension, attention, honesty, and reliability. We then explore differences in these data quality aspects between online platforms (where participants choose tasks) and online panels (where respondents are assigned surveys). We find that most audiences suffer concerning deficits, especially in comprehension and attention, except for Prolific, which appears superior to the other options on these and other parameters. The disappointing results from the other platforms and panels should alarm researchers who may be aggregating over serious noise in the quality of their results. Additionally, we provide a framework for ongoing investigation into the ever-changing nature of what aspects of data quality are important to researchers, and how the evolving set of audiences performs on these key aspects.

 

 
Peer, E. & Feldman, Y., 2021. Honesty Pledges for the Behaviorally-based Regulation of Dishonesty. Journal of European Public Policy. Available at: . Publisher's VersionAbstract

 

A common regulatory dilemma is determining how much trust authorities can place in people’s self-reports, especially in contexts with an incentive to cheat. In such contexts, regulators are typically risk averse and do not readily confer trust, resulting in excessive requirements when applying for permits, licenses, etc. Studies in behavioral ethics have suggested that asking people to ex-ante pledge to behave ethically can reduce their level of dishonesty and noncompliance. However, pledges might also backfire by allowing more people to cheat with no real sanctions. Additionally, pledges’ effects have almost always been studied in one-shot decision making without sanctions. We explore pledges’ potential effects by manipulating whether pledges were accompanied by sanctions (fines) and testing their impact on sequential, repeated ethical decisions. We find that pledges can considerably and consistently reduce dishonesty, and this effect is not crowded out by the presence of fines. Furthermore, pledges also affect “brazen liars” who cheat to a large extent and also those who score low on tendency to follow rules and norms. We conclude that pledges could be an effective tool for the behavioral regulation of dishonesty, to reduce regulatory burden, and to build a more trusting relationship between government and the public.

 

2020
Tikotsky, A., Pe'er, E. & Feldman, Y., 2020. Which nudges do businesses like? Managers’ attitudes towards nudges directed at their business or at their customers. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization. Available at: . Publisher's VersionAbstract

Behaviorally informed policies of interventions in choice architecture are growingly used to nudge people towards socially desirable behaviors. While consumers are usually the target of those nudges, businesses often serve as “nudging agents" on behalf of government regulation, or may be the target of governmental nudges themselves. Businesses’ support of such behavioral policies might be critical for their implementation, but the perceptions of managers towards nudges has never been directly assessed. We distinguish between government-to-business (G2B) nudges vs. government-to-business-to-consumer (G2B2C) nudges and provide first evidence of business managers’ attitudes towards such interventions. We discover an overall high level of support for nudges, and in particular for G2B nudges, with variations between types of nudges, the domain that they operate in, and whether they benefit the business or the consumer.

Peer, E. et al., 2020. Nudge Me Right: Personalizing Online Security Nudges to People's Decision-Making Styles. Computers in Human Behavior, 109, p.106347. Available at: . Publisher's VersionAbstract

Nudges are simple and effective means to help people make decisions that could benefit themselves or society. However, effects of nudges are limited to local maxima, as they are almost always designed with the “average” person in mind, instead of being customized to different individuals. Such “nudge personalization” has been advocated before, but its actual potency and feasibility has never been systematically investigated. Using the ubiquitous domain of online password nudges as our testbed, we present a novel approach that utilizes people’s decision-making style to personalize the online nudge they receive. In two large-scale studies, we show how and when personalized nudges can lead to considerably stronger and more secure passwords, compared to administering “one-size-fits-all” nudges. We discuss the implications of this findings and how more efforts by researchers and policy-makers should and could be made to guarantee that each individual is nudged in a way most right for them. 

2019
Frik, A. et al., 2019. A Promise Is A Promise: The Effect Of Commitment Devices On Computer Security Intentions. CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems Proceedings. Available at: . Publisher's Version
Peer, E. et al., 2019. Do minorities like nudges? The role of group norms in attitudes towards behavioral policy. Judgment and Decision Making.Abstract

Attitudes of public groups towards behavioral policy interventions (or nudges) can be important for both the policy makers who design and deploy nudges, and to researchers who try to understand when and why some nudges are supported while others are not. Until now, research on public attitudes towards nudges has focused on either state- or country-level comparisons, or on correlations with individual-level traits, and has neglected to study how different social groups (such as minorities) might view nudges. Using a large and representative sample, we tested the attitudes of two distinct minority groups in Israel (Israeli Arabs and Ultra-Orthodox Jews), and discovered that nudges that operated against a minority group’s held social norms, promoting a more general societal goal not aligned with the group’s norms, were often less supported by minorities. Contrary to expectations, these differences could not be explained by differences in trust in the government applying these nudges. We discuss implications for public policy and for the research and applications of behavioral interventions.

Vosgerau, J. & Peer, E., 2019. Extreme malleability of preferences: Absolute preference sign changes under uncertainty. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 32(1), pp.38-46. Available at: . Publisher's VersionAbstract

Malleability of preferences is a central tenet of behavioral decision theory. How malleable preferences really are, however, is a topic of debate. Do preference reversals imply preference construction? We argue that to claim preferences are construed, a demonstration of more extreme preference malleability than simple preference reversals is required: absolute preference sign changes within participants. If respondents value a prospect positively in 1 condition but negatively in a different condition, preferences cannot be considered stable. Such absolute preference sign changes are possible under uncertainty. In 2 incentive‐compatible experiments, we found participants were willing to pay to take part in a gamble and also demanded to be compensated to take part in a subsequent gamble with identical outcomes and probabilities. Such absolute preference sign changes within participants led to simultaneous risk aversion and risk seeking for the same risky prospect, suggesting that, at least in the domain of risky decisions, consumers' preferences are indeed malleable and construed.

2018
Frik, A. et al., 2018. Better Late(r) than Never: Increasing Cyber-Security Compliance by Reducing Present Bias. WEIS. Available at: . Publisher's Version
Peer, E., Samat, S. & Acquisti, A., 2018. Can Digital Face-Morphs Influence Attitudes and Online Behaviors?. Symposium on Usable Privacy and Security (SOUPS). Available at: . Publisher's VersionAbstract

Self-images are among the most prevalent forms of content shared on social media streams. Face-morphs are images digitally created by combining facial pictures of different individuals. In the case of self-morphs, a person’s own picture is combined with that of another individual. Prior research has shown that even when individuals do not recognize themselves in self-morphs, they tend to trust self-morphed faces more, and judge them more favorably. Thus, self-morphs may be used online as covert forms of targeted marketing–for instance, using consumers’ pictures from social media streams to create self-morphs, and inserting the resulting self-morphs in promotional campaigns targeted at those consumers. The usage of this type of personal data for highly targeted influence without individuals' awareness, and the type of opaque effect such artifacts may have on individuals' attitudes and behaviors, raise potential issues of consumer privacy and autonomy. However, no research to date has examined the feasibility of using self-morphs for such applications. Research on self-morphs has focused on artificial laboratory settings, raising questions regarding the practical, in-the-wild applicability of reported self-morph effects. In three experiments, we examine whether self-morphs could affect individuals' attitudes or even promote products/services, using a combination of experimental designs and dependent variables. Across the experiments, we test both designs and variables that had been used in previous research in this area and new ones that had not. Questioning prior research, however, we find no evidence that end-users react more …

Adjerid, I., Peer, E. & Acquisti, A., 2018. Beyond the Privacy Paradox: Objective versus Relative Risk in Privacy Decision Making. MIS Quarterly, 42(2), pp.465 - A5. Available at: . Publisher's VersionAbstract
The article presents a study on both the objective and relative risks involved with privacy decision making. Topics include the impact of changes in the objective risk of disclosure and the impact of changes in the relative perceptions of risk of disclosure on hypothetical and actual consumer privacy choices, a decrease in objective risk going from hypothetical to actual choice settings, and an increase in relative risk going from hypothetical to actual choice settings.
Hammer, R., Peer, E. & Babad, E., 2018. Faculty attitudes about student evaluations and their relations to self-image as teacher. Social Psychology of Education, 21(3), pp.517 - 537. Available at: . Publisher's VersionAbstract
With the worldwide implementation of students' evaluation of teaching (SET), faculty attitudes and trust in students' feedback as well as possible defensive (i.e., self-protective) motivations seem most relevant to the facilitation of the primary organizational goal of SET, namely, teaching improvement. A questionnaire-administered to 2241 faculty members of all ranks in two dozen varied institutions-measured positive attitudes and trust, on the one hand, and beliefs in salient negative faculty SET myths, on the other hand. The most widely-held negative attitudes concerned student fallibilities: vindictiveness; lack of maturity; and negative evaluations of low-achieving students. Despite believing in myths, more than half of the respondents reported trusting SET, thought that it accurately reflected their teaching performance, and considered SET-based feedback useful. A derived index comparing self-evaluations to reported students' evaluations demonstrated that more than a third of th
2017
Gamliel, E. & Peer, E., 2017. The Average Fuel-Efficiency Fallacy: Overestimation of Average Fuel Efficiency and How It Can Lead to Biased Decisions. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, (2), p.435. Available at: . Publisher's VersionAbstract
People falsely believe that equal increases in vehicles' fuel efficiency (e.g., miles per gallon (MPG)) will result in equal fuel savings. Whereas previous research on this “MPG illusion” has focused on people's biased choices of upgrading vehicle models, it has not examined a more common situation, namely, estimating a given vehicle's fuel efficiency based on the average of two efficiency values (e.g., in the city and on highways). In such situations, we find an additional bias in people's judgment and choice, the average fuel‐efficiency fallacy, in which people falsely believe that the combined fuel efficiency (e.g., of city and highway MPG) is a simple—instead of a harmonic—mean of the two values. Owing to the curvilinear relationship between fuel efficiency and fuel consumption, the combined fuel‐efficiency value would always be lower than the simple average, resulting in consistent overestimations of the actual fuel efficiency. In a series of studies, we demonstrate how this fallacy of overestimating combined fuel efficiency leads to suboptimal choices between vehicles. In addition, we find that the solution prescribed for the MPG illusion—using gallons per 100 miles—does reduce, but not eliminate, the average fuel‐efficiency fallacy, and that comprehension of the gallons per 100 miles measure is a precursory condition for this nudge to have any effect.
Peer, E. et al., 2017. Beyond the Turk: Alternative platforms for crowdsourcing behavioral research. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, p.153. Available at: . Publisher's VersionAbstract
The success of Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) as an online research platform has come at a price: MTurk has suffered from slowing rates of population replenishment, and growing participant non-naivety. Recently, a number of alternative platforms have emerged, offering capabilities similar to MTurk but providing access to new and more naive populations. After surveying several options, we empirically examined two such platforms, CrowdFlower (CF) and Prolific Academic (ProA). In two studies, we found that participants on both platforms were more naive and less dishonest compared to MTurk participants. Across the three platforms, CF provided the best response rate, but CF participants failed more attention-check questions and did not reproduce known effects replicated on ProA and MTurk. Moreover, ProA participants produced data quality that was higher than CF's and comparable to MTurk's. ProA and CF participants were also much more diverse than participants from MTurk.
2016
Egelman, S., Harbach, M. & Peer, E., 2016. Behavior Ever Follows Intention?: A Validation of the Security Behavior Intentions Scale (SeBIS). Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems Proceedings, p.5257. Available at: . Publisher's VersionAbstract

The Security Behavior Intentions Scale (SeBIS) measures the computer security attitudes of end-users. Because intentions are a prerequisite for planned behavior, the scale could therefore be useful for predicting users' computer security behaviors. We performed three experiments to identify correlations between each of SeBIS's four sub-scales and relevant computer security behaviors. We found that testing high on the awareness sub-scale correlated with correctly identifying a phishing website; testing high on the passwords sub-scale correlated with creating passwords that could not be quickly cracked; testing high on the updating sub-scale correlated with applying software updates; and testing high on the securement sub-scale correlated with smartphone lock screen usage (e.g., PINs). Our results indicate that SeBIS predicts certain computer security behaviors and that it is a reliable and valid tool that should be used in future research.

Peer, E. & Acquisti, A., 2016. The impact of reversibility on the decision to disclose personal information. Journal of Consumer Marketing, (6). Available at: . Publisher's VersionAbstract

This paper aims to examine how reversibility in disclosing personal information – that is, having (vs not having) to option to later revise or retract personal information – can impact consumers’ willingness to divulge personal information. Three studies examined how informing consumers they may (reversible condition) or may not (irreversible condition) revise their personal information in the future affected their propensity to disclose personal information, compared to a control condition. Study 1 (which included three experiments with different time intervals between initial and revised disclosure) showed that consumers disclose less in both the reversible and irreversible conditions, compared to the control condition. Studies 2 and 3 showed that this is because consumers treat reversibility as a cue to the sensitivity of the information they are asked to divulge, and that leads them to disclose less when reversibility or irreversibility is made explicitly salient beforehand. As many marketers are interested in hoarding consumers’ personal information, privacy advocates call for methods that would ensure careful and well-informed disclosure. Offering reversibility to a decision to disclose personal information, or merely pointing out the irreversibility of that decision, can make consumers reevaluate the sensitivity of the situation, leading to more careful disclosures. Although previous research on reversibility in consumer behavior focused on product return policies and showed that reversibility increases purchases, none have studied how reversibility affects self-disclosure and how it can decrease it.

2015
Egelman, S. & Peer, E., 2015. The myth of the average user: Improving privacy and security systems through individualization. Proceedings of the 2015 New Security Paradigms Workshop. Available at: . Publisher's VersionAbstract

While individual differences in decision-making have been examined within the social sciences for several decades, they have only recently begun to be applied by computer scientists to examine privacy and security attitudes (and ultimately behaviors). Specifically, several researchers have shown how different online privacy decisions are correlated with the "Big Five" personality traits. In this paper, we show that the five factor model is actually a weak predictor of privacy attitudes, and that other well-studied individual differences in the psychology literature are much stronger predictors. Based on this result, we introduce the new paradigm of psychographic targeting of privacy and security mitigations: we believe that the next frontier in privacy and security research will be to tailor mitigations to users' individual differences. We explore the extensive work on choice architecture and "nudges," and discuss the possible ways it could be leveraged to improve security outcomes by personalizing privacy and security mitigations to specific user traits.